Materialist conceptions about the immediate seat of conscious-ness

Authors

DOI:

https://doi.org/10.11606/issn.2178-6224v15i1p93-136

Keywords:

Primary consciousness, Neural correlate of consciousness, Holism, Materialism, Localizationism

Abstract

This paper studies, from a materialist point of view, the debate concerning what is the physical seat of “primary consciousness”, that is, the most basic form of consciousness involving sensations and emotions. We examine historically the problem of whether such a sensorium is located in a small region of the brain or whether consciousness is distributed in a holistic way. Emphasis is given to the writings of the neurophysiologists of the 1950’s, divided between the claim that the seat of primary consciousness is subcortical, and more holistic views like that of the homogeneity thesis (functionalism). One attempts to explain the transition that took place in the end of the 1960’s, and the privileged role that was given to the neocortex. The philosophical aim of this paper is to recover the importance of the subcorticalist thesis.

References

ALKIRE, Michael T.; HUDETZ, Anthony G. & TONONI, Giulio. Consciousness and anesthesia. Science, 322: 876-880, 2008.

ARMSTRONG, David M. Three types of consciousness [Com-mentary]. Pp. 235-241, in: Ciba Foundation (org.). Brain and mind. Amsterdam: Excerpta Medica, 1979.

BAARS, Bernard J. & GAGE, Nichole M. Cognition, brain and con-sciousness: introduction to cognitive neuroscience. 2a ed. Burlington (MA): Academic Press, 2010.

BAILEY, Percival. The seat of the soul. Perspectives in Biology and Medi-cine, 2: 417-441, 1959.

BAILEY, Percival. Cortex and mind. Pp. 3-14, in: Scher, J. M. (org.). Theories of the mind. New York: Free Press, 1962.

BARRON, Andrew B. & KLEIN, Colin. What insects can tell us about the origins of consciousness. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, U.S.A., 113: 4900-4908, 2016.

BOGEN, Joseph E. On the neurophysiology of consciousness. I. An overview. II. Constraining the semantic problem. Consciousness and Cognition, 4: 52-62, 137-158, 1995.

BORING, Edwin G. Sensation and perception in the history of experimental psychology. New York: Appleton-Century, 1942.

CABANAC, M.; CABANAC, A.J. & PARENT, A. The emergence of consciousness in phylogeny. Behavioral Brain Research, 198: 267-272, 2009.

CAIRNS, Hugh. Disturbances of consciousness with lesions of the brain-stem and diencephalon. Brain, 75: 109-146, 1952.

CARPENTER, William B. Principles of human physiology. 6a ed. Lon-don: John Churchill, 1864.

CARTER, Rita; ALDRIDGE, S.; PAGE, M. & PARKER, S. O livro do cérebro, 4 vols. Trad. P. Frances. Edição especial da revista Mente & Cérebro. São Paulo: Duetto, 2009.

CHAPMAN, Loring F. & WOLFF, Harold G. The cerebral hemi-spheres and the highest integrative functions of man. A. M. A. Archives of Neurology, 1: 357-424, 1959.

COBB, Stanley. On the nature and locus of mind. A. M. A. Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry, 67: 172-177.

CRICK, Francis C. Function of the thalamic reticular complex: the searchlight hypothesis. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences U.S.A., 81: 4586-4590, 1984.

CRICK, Francis C. & KOCH, Christof. What is the function of the claustrum? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 360: 1271-1279, 2005.

DAMÁSIO, António. A neurobiology for consciousness. Pp. 111-120, in: Metzinger, T. (org.). Neural correlates of consciousness: empirical and conceptual questions. Cambridge (MA): MIT Press, 2000.

DELAFRESNAYE, J.F.; ADRIAN, Edgar D.; BREMER, Frederic & JASPER, Herbert H. (orgs.). Brain mechanisms and consciousness. Oxford: Blackwell, 1954.

DESCARTES, René. As paixões da alma [1649]. Trad. J. Guinsburg & B. Prado Jr. Pp. 213-294, in: Os Pensadores. 2ª ed. São Paulo: Abril Cultural, 1979.

DRESLER, M.; WEHRLE, R.; SPOORMAKER, V.I.; KOCH, S.P.; HOLSBOER, F.; STEIGER, A.; OBRIG, H., SÄMANN, P.G. & CZISCH, M. Neural correlates of dream lucidity obtained from contrasting lucid versus non-lucid REM sleep: a combined EEG/fMRI case study. Sleep, 35: 1017-1020, 2012.

EDELMAN, Gerald M. Bright air, brilliant fire: on the matter of the mind. New York: Basic Books, 1992.

ECCLES, John C. The neurophysiological basis of mind: the principles of neurophysiology. Oxford: Clarendon, 1953.

EVANS, Cedric O. The subject of consciousness. London: George Allen & Unwin, 1970.

FELTEN, David L.; SHETTY, Anil N. Netter atlas de neurociência. Ilustrações Frank H. Netter. Trad. R. R. Santos. Rio de Janeiro: Guanabara Koogan, 2009.

FEINDEL, William. Mind, consciousness, and the neurosurgeon. Pp. 49-66, in: Awad, I. A. (org.). Philosophy of neurological surgery. Park Ridge (IL): American Association of Neurological Sur-geons, 1995.

FESSARD, Alfred E. Mechanisms of nervous integration and con-scious experience. Pp. 200-236, in: Delafresnaye et al., op. cit., 1954.

FOERSTER, Otfrid. Über die Bedeutung und Reichweite des Loka-lisationsprinzips im Nervensystem. Pp. 117-211, in: Géronne A. (org.), Sechsundvierzigster Kongress. Verhandlungen der Deutschen Gesell-schaft für Innere Medizin. Munich: Bergmann, 1934.

FRENCH, John D. Brain lesions associated with prolonged unconsciousness. A. M. A. Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry, 68: 727-740, 1952.

GAZZANIGA, Michael S. The consciousness instinct: unraveling the mystery of how the brain makes the mind. New York: Farrar, Straus & Giroux, 2018.

GOLDSTEIN, Kurt. Der Aufbau der Organismus. Haia: Nijhoff, 1934. Tradução para o inglês: The organism. New York: Zone Books, 1995.

HAGMANN, Patric; CAMMOUN, L.; GIGANDET, X.; MEULI, R.; HONEY, C. J.; WEDEEN, V. J. & SPORNS, O. Mapping the structural core of human cerebral cortex. PLoS Biology, 6: 1479-1493, 2008.

HOBSON, J. Allan. Toward a cellular neurophysiology of the reticular formation: conceptual and methodological milestones. Pp. 7-29, in: HOBSON, J. A. & BRAZIER, M. A. B. (orgs.). The reticular formation revisited: specifying function for a nonspecific system. New York: Raven Press, 1980.

HODGSON, Shadworth H. The philosophy of reflection. London: Longmans, Green and Co., 1878.

JACKSON, John Hughlings. Remarks on evolution and dissolution of the nervous system. Journal of Mental Science, 33: 25-48, 1887.

JAMES, William. The principles of psychology. London: Macmillan, 1890.

JUNG, Richard. Correlation of bioelectrical and autonomic phe-nomena with alterations of consciousness and arousal in man. Pp. 310-344, in: Delafresnaye et al., op. cit., 1954.

KANDEL, Eric R. Em busca da memória [2006]. Trad. R. Rubino. São Paulo: Cia. das Letras, 2009.

KUHN, Thomas S. A estrutura das revoluções científicas [1962]. Trad. B. V. Boeira & N. Boeira. São Paulo: Perspectiva, 1978.

KOCH, Christof; MASSIMINI, Marcello; BOLY, Melanie & TONONI, Giulio. Neural correlates of consciousness: progress and problems. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 17: 307-322, 2016.

LABERGE, David L. Attention. Psychological Science, 1: 156-162, 1990.

LE BEAU, Jacques. Localisation cérébrale de la conscience. Revue Canadienne de Biologie, 1: 134-56, 1942.

LEDOUX, Joseph. Rethinking the emotional brain. Neuron, 73: 653-676, 2012.

LE GROS CLARK, Wilfrid E. The structure of the brain and the process of thinking. Pp. 12-24, in: LASLETT, P. (org.). The physi-cal basis of mind. Oxford: Basil Blackwell, 1950.

LLINÁS, Rodolfo R. & PARÉ, Denis. Of dreaming and wakeful-ness. Neuroscience, 44: 521-35, 1991.

LOW, Philip; PANKSEPP, Jaak; REISS, Diana; EDELMAN, Da-vid; VAN SWINDEREN, Bruno & KOCH, Christof. The Cambridge Declaration on Consciousness. Disponível online: http://fcmconference.org/img/CambridgeDeclarationOnConsciousness.pdf

MARSHALL, Louise H. & MAGOUN, Horace W. Discoveries in the human brain: neuroscience prehistory, brain structure, and function. New York: Springer, 1998.

MASHOUR, George A. & ALKIRE, Michael T. Consciousness, anesthesia, and the thalamocortical system. Anesthesiology, 118: 13-15, 2013.

MERKER, Björn. Consciousness without a cerebral cortex: a chal-lenge for neuroscience and medicine. Target article, commen-taries and author’s response. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30: 63-134, 2007.

MESULAM, M.-Marsel. Principles of behavioral and cognitive neurology. 2a ed. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2000.

MEYERS, Russell. Dandy’s striatal theory of “the center of con-sciousness”: surgical evidence and logical analysis indicating its improbability. A.M.A. Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry, 65: 659-671, 1951.

MILL, John Stuart. A system of logic, ratiocinative and inductive. Vol. 1. London: John Parker, 1843.

MORABITO, Carmela. David Ferrier and Luigi Luciani on the localization of brain functions. Physis, 36: 387-405, 1999.

MORUZZI, Giuseppe & MAGOUN, Horace W. Brain stem re-ticular formation and activation of EEG. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 1: 455-473, 1949.

MUMFORD, David. On the computational architecture of the neocortex. I. The role of the thalamo-cortical loop. Biological Cy-bernetics, 65: 135-145, 1991.

NEWMAN, James. Thalamic contributions to attention and con-sciousness. Consciousness and Cognition, 4: 142-193, 1995.

NIELSEN, Johannes M. & THOMPSON, George N. The en-grammes of psychiatry. Springfield (IL): Charles Thomas, 1947.

PANKSEPP, Jaak. Toward a general psychobiological theory of emotions. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 5: 407-467, 1982.

PANKSEPP, Jaak. Forward: the MacLean legacy and some modern trends in emotion research. Pp. iv-xxvii, in: Cory, Jr., G. A. & Gardner, Jr., R. (orgs.). The evolutionary neuroethology of Paul MacLean: convergences and frontiers. Westport (CT): Praeger, 2002.

PARVIZI, Josef & DAMÁSIO, António R. Consciousness and the brainstem. Cognition, 79: 135-160, 2001.

PENFIELD, Wilder. The cerebral cortex in man. I. The cerebral cortex and consciousness. Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry, 40(3): 417-442, 1938.

PENFIELD, Wilder. The centrencephalic integrating system. Brain, 81: 231-234, 1958.

PENFIELD, Wilder. O mistério da mente [1975]. São Paulo: Atheneu/Edusp, 1983.

PESSOA JR., Osvaldo. A consciência enquanto observação do cérebro. Pp. 201-219, in: Ferreira, F. R. M.; Haddad Jr., H.; No-gueira, M. I. & Pessoa Jr., O. (orgs.). História e filosofia da neurociên-cia. São Paulo: Liber Ars, 2015.

PIÉRON, Henri. De l’apport actuel de la neurophysiologie à la psychologie. L’année psychologique, 53: 279-319, 1953.

PROCHASKA, Georg. A dissertation on the functions of the nervous system [1784]. Trad. T. Laycock. Pp. 363-450, in Unzer, J. A. & Pro-chaska, G. ‘The principles of physiology’ and ‘A dissertation on the functions of the nervous system’. London: Sydenham Society, 1851.

REICHARDT, Martin. Hirnstamm und Psychiatrie. Monatsschrift für Psychiatrie und Neurologie, 68: 470-506, 1928. Disponibilizado onli-ne em duas partes pela European Neurology, 2008.

REES, Geraint; KREIMAN, Gabriel & KOCH, Christof. Neural correlates of consciousness in humans. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3: 261-270, 2002.

ROGET, Peter M. Animal and vegetable physiology considered with reference to natural theology. Vol. II. London: W. Pickering, 1834.

ROSENTHAL, David M. A theory of consciousness. Pp. 729-754, in: BLOCK, N.; FLANAGAN, O. & GUZELDERE, G. (orgs.). The nature of consciousness: Philosophical debates. Cambridge (MA): MIT Press, 1992.

ROTH, Gerhard. The evolution and ontogeny of consciousness. Pp. 76-97, in: METZINGER, T. (org.). Neural correlates of con-sciousness: empirical and conceptual questions. Cambridge (MA): MIT Press, 2000.

SAHRAIE, A.; WEISKRANTZ, L.; BARBUR, J. L.; SIMMONS, A.; WILLIAMS, S. C. R. & BRAMMER, M. J. Pattern of neu-ronal activity associated with conscious and unconscious pro-cessing of visual signals. Proceedings of the National Academy of Scienc-es U.S.A., 94: 9406-9411, 1997.

SCHÄFER, Edward A. The cerebral cortex. Pp. 697-782, in: Schäfer, E.A. (org.). Text-book of physiology. Vol. 2. Edinburgh: Pentland, 1900.

SCHEIBEL, Madge E. & SCHEIBEL, Arnold B. The anatomy of constancy. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 290: 421-435, 1977.

SCHILLER, Francis. Consciousness reconsidered. A. M. A. Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry, 67: 199-227.

SCHMITT, L. I.; WIMMER, R. D.; NAKAJIMA, M.; HAPP, M.; MOFAKHAM, S. & HALASSA, M. M. Thalamic amplification of cortical connectivity sustains attentional control. Nature, 545: 219-223, 2017.

SEARLE, John. A redescoberta da mente [1992]. São Paulo: Martins Fontes, 1997.

SHIGENO, S.; ANDREWS, P. L. R.; PONTE, G. & FIORITO, G. Cefalopod brains: an overview of current knowledge to facilitate comparison with vertebrates. Frontiers in Physiology, 9: (article 952) 1-16, 2018.

SNEDDON, Lynne U. Pain perception in fish: evidence and impli-cations for the use of fish. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 18: 209-229, 2011.

SOKOLOV, Eugene. Psychophysiology of consciousness. Trad. do russo por A. Chernorizov; K. Chernorizov & D. Bowden. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2013.

SPENCER, Herbert. Principles of psychology. Vol. 1. 3a ed. New York: D. Appleton & Co., 1890.

SPERRY, Roger. Consciousness, personal identity and the divided brain. Neuropsychologia, 22: 661-673, 1984.

THOMPSON, Robert. (1993). Centrencephalic theory, the general learning system, and subcortical dementia. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 702: 197-223, 1993.

TONONI, Giulio & EDELMAN, Gerald M. Consciousness and complexity. Science, 282: 1846-1851, 1998.

TONONI, Giulio & KOCH, Christof. The neural correlates of consciousness: an update. Annals of the New York Academy of Sci-ences, 1124: 239-261, 2008.

TROLAND, Leonard T. Principles of psychophysiology. Vol. III: Cerebra-tion and action. New York: Greenwood Press, 1932.

VELLY, L. J; REY, M. F.; BRUDER, N. J.; GOUVITSOS, F. A.; WITJAS, T.; REGIS, J. M.; PERAGUT, J. C. & GOUIN, F. M. Differential dynamic of action on cortical and subcortical struc-tures of anesthetic agents during induction of anesthesia. Anesthe-siology, 107: 202-212, 2007.

WALSHE, Francis M. R. The brain-stem conceived as the “highest level” of function in the nervous system; with particular refer-ence to the “automatic apparatus” of Carpenter (1850) and to the “centrencephalic integrating system” of Penfield. Brain, 80: 510-539, 1957.

WARD, Lawrence M. The thalamic dynamic core theory of con-sciousness. Consciousness and Cognition, 20: 464-486, 2011.

WATT, D. The centrencephalon and thalamocortical integration: neglected contributions of periaqueductal gray. Consciousness and Emotion, 1: 91-114, 2000.

WEISKRANTZ, Lawrence. Some contributions of neuropsycholo-gy of vision and memory to the problem of consciousness. Pp. 183-99, in: MARCEL, A. J. & BISIACH, E. (eds.). Consciousness in contemporary science. Oxford: Clarendon, 1988.

ZEKI, S. & BARTELS, A. Toward a theory of visual conscious-ness. Consciousness and Cognition, 8: 225-259, 1999.

Published

2020-11-17

Issue

Section

Papers

How to Cite

Materialist conceptions about the immediate seat of conscious-ness. Filosofia e História da Biologia , [S. l.], v. 15, n. 1, p. 93–136, 2020. DOI: 10.11606/issn.2178-6224v15i1p93-136. Disponível em: https://periodicos.usp.br/fhb/article/view/fhb-v15-n1-06.. Acesso em: 28 jun. 2024.